早期双语教育

早期双语教育

双语是指在日常生活中使用两种语言的能力。 双语是种很普遍的能力,并且在世界许多地方都在上升,世界上至少有三分之一的人是双语或多语的。
 
对早期双语主义的态度通常基于误解,而不是科学。 在这里,我们想帮助回答父母提出的六个最常见的问题。 并尝试证明接纳说中文的互惠生的好处。

请点击问题以获取答案 (答案的中文译文暂不提供):

1. 双语的孩子在语言学习上受多的干扰吗?

One misunderstood behavior and evidence for confusion, is when bilingual children mix words from two languages in the same sentence. This is known as code mixing. In fact, code mixing is a normal part of bilingual development, and bilingual children actually have good reasons to code mix (Person, 2008). One reason some children code mix is that it happens frequently in their language communities—children imitate what they hear adults around them do (Comeau, Genesee & Lapaquette, 2003). A second reason is that young bilinguals are sometimes limited in their vocabulary, just like other young monolinguals. Rather than being a sign of confusion, code mixing can be seen as a sign of bilingual children’s ingenuity.

What about bilingual infants? Again, the research is clear: bilingual infants readily distinguish their two languages and show no evidence of confusion. Languages differ on many dimensions. Even if you don’t speak Russian or Mandarin, you can likely tell one from the other. Infants are very sensitive to these perceptual differences, and are particularly attuned to a language’s rhythm. Recent research has shown that 4-month-old monolingual and bilingual infants can discriminate silent talking faces speaking different languages (Weikum et al., 2007). However, by 8 months of age, only bilinguals are still sensitive to the distinction, while monolinguals stop paying attention to subtle variations in facial movements (Sebastián – Galleés, Albareda-Castellot, Weikum, & Werker, 2012); Weikum et al., 2007) Rather than being confused, it seems that bilingual infants are more sensitive at distinguishing their languages.

2. 双语使孩子们变得更聪明吗?

One of the most important benefits of early bilingualism is often taken for granted: bilingual children will know multiple languages which is important for daily life, connecting to family culture and history, and making friends from different cultures. However, beyond all these obvious benefits, researchers have investigated whether bilingualism leads to other non-linguistic advantages.

Scientific research has also shown that bilingual children have enhanced social understanding and bilinguals have shown some cognitive advantages such as switching between activities and inhibiting previously learned responses (Akhtar & Menjivar, 2012).

3. “一人一语”的方式对双语的孩子更有利吗?

“One-person-one-language,” a strategy first recommended over 100 years ago (Ronjat, 1913), has been proven false. It is neither necessary nor sufficient for successful bilingual acquisition. There is no evidence that bilingual children are confused by early bilingualism. 

Several other factors have proven to be important to early bilingual development. Among them, quantity and quality. Bilingual children who hear a large amount of a particular language learn more words and grammar in that language ( Hoff et al., 2012; Person & Fernaández, 1994), and show more efficient processing of that language (Conboy & Mills, 2006; Hurtado, Grüter, Marchman & Fernald, 2013; Marchman, Fernald & Hurtado, 2010). High quality language exposure involves social interaction. For example, infants do not readily learn language from television (DeLoache etal., 2010; Kuhl, Tsao & Liu, 2003), and low-quality television viewing in infancy has been linked to smaller vocabulary in bilingual toddlers (Hudon, Fennell & Hoftyzer, 2013). Opportunities to interact with multiple and different speakers has been linked to vocabulary learning in bilingual toddlers (Pace & Hoff, 2010).

These factors can lead some families to consider the one-person-one-language approach, while other families use another approach. Relatively balanced exposure to the two languages is most likely the best way to promote successful acquisition of both of the languages ( Thordardottir, 2011). So the best answer to this question is that parents should use whatever strategy promotes high-quality and high-quantity exposure to their child’s language skills.            

4. 父母应该避免混淆两种语言吗?

Mixing languages together is also known as ‘code mixing’ and is a normal part of being a bilingual and interacting with other bilingual speakers (Poplack, 1980). Studies are beginning to reveal that bilingual children as young as 20-months are able to understand code-mixed sentences, and show similar processing patterns as bilingual adults (Byers-Heinlein, 2013). This would suggest that bilinguals are able to cope with code mixing from an early age. It has also been suggested that while code mixing might make word learning initially difficult, it is possible that practice switching back and forth between the languages leads to cognitive benefits later in life (Byers- Heinlein, 2013).

5. 越早越好?

Many parents are convinced about the concept of a ‘critical period’ for language acquisition: the idea that humans are not capable of mastering a new language after reaching a certain age. Researchers disagree about whether a critical period exists at all, and they disagree about when this critical period may occur. Proposals range from age 5 to 15 (Krashen, 1973; John son & Newport, 1989; Lenneber, 1967). However, the majority of them do agree ‘the earlier, the better’. There may not be a sharp turn for the worse at any point in development, but there is an incremental decline in language learning abilities with age (Birdsong & Molis, 2001; Hakuta, Bialystok, Wiley, 2003).  

This point is best understood as an interaction between biological and environmental factors. Young children experience a very rich language environment during the first years of life in most cultures and families. Older children and adults do not usually have the same amount of time to devote to language learning, and they do not usually experience the advantage of fun, constant, one-on-one interaction with native speakers. 

Applied to bilingualism, simultaneous bilinguals (acquiring two languages from birth) do display fairly robust advantages over sequential bilinguals (first language followed by second language). They tend to have better accents, more diversified vocabulary, higher grammatical proficiency, and greater skill in real-time language processing (Lew – Williams & Fernald, 2007, 2010).


However, parents should not lose their hope if they have not exposed their children to each language right from birth. There are many other ways to foster bilingual development. Such as hiring bilingual caretakers for your children or sending your children to language immersion programs in elementary school.

6. 双语儿童更容易发生学习语言的困难,延迟或障碍吗?

Bilingual children are not more likely than monolingual children to have difficulties with language, to show delays in learning, or to be diagnosed with a language disorder (Paradis, Genesee & Crago, 2010; Petitoo & Holowka, 2002). It is the perception of their parents. 

If in the parents believe there is a delay or even disorder in language learning of their children, consult a pediatrician or if possible, a speech-language pathologist with expertise in bilingualism. If we measure bilinguals using a monolingual measure, we are more likely to find false evidence of delay. Fortunately, researchers and clinicians are now developing bilingual-specific measures that paint a more accurate picture of bilinguals’ global language competence.

Research demonstrates that we need to reshape our views of early bilingualism: children are born ready to learn languages of their environments without confusion or delay (Werker & Byers-Heinlein, 2008). At Au Pair Bureau HELDER, we promote bilingualism and multicultural proficiency among our children.